class: center, middle, inverse, title-slide # Distributions ## DATA 606 - Statistics & Probability for Data Analytics ### Jason Bryer, Ph.D., Angela Lui, Ph.D., and George Hagstrom, Ph.D. ### October 2, 2024 --- # One Minute Paper Results .pull-left[ **What was the most important thing you learned during this class?** <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-2-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> ] .pull-right[ **What important question remains unanswered for you?** <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-3-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> ] --- # Coin Tosses Revisited ``` r coins <- sample(c(-1,1), 100, replace=TRUE) plot(1:length(coins), cumsum(coins), type='l') abline(h=0) ``` <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-4-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> ``` r cumsum(coins)[length(coins)] ``` ``` ## [1] 4 ``` --- class: font120 # Many Random Samples ``` r samples <- rep(NA, 1000) for(i in seq_along(samples)) { coins <- sample(c(-1,1), 100, replace=TRUE) samples[i] <- cumsum(coins)[length(coins)] } head(samples, n = 15) ``` ``` ## [1] -2 2 -6 6 -20 16 -6 -2 -16 -4 0 -2 -2 -12 2 ``` --- # Histogram of Many Random Samples ``` r hist(samples) ``` <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-6-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Properties of Distribution ``` r (m.sam <- mean(samples)) ``` ``` ## [1] -0.342 ``` ``` r (s.sam <- sd(samples)) ``` ``` ## [1] 10.11164 ``` --- # Properties of Distribution (cont.) ``` r within1sd <- samples[samples >= m.sam - s.sam & samples <= m.sam + s.sam] length(within1sd) / length(samples) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.672 ``` ``` r within2sd <- samples[samples >= m.sam - 2 * s.sam & samples <= m.sam + 2* s.sam] length(within2sd) / length(samples) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.954 ``` ``` r within3sd <- samples[samples >= m.sam - 3 * s.sam & samples <= m.sam + 3 * s.sam] length(within3sd) / length(samples) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.996 ``` --- # Standard Normal Distribution `$$f\left( x|\mu ,\sigma \right) =\frac { 1 }{ \sigma \sqrt { 2\pi } } { e }^{ -\frac { { \left( x-\mu \right) }^{ 2 } }{ { 2\sigma }^{ 2 } } }$$` ``` r x <- seq(-4,4,length=200); y <- dnorm(x,mean=0, sd=1) plot(x, y, type = "l", lwd = 2, xlim = c(-3.5,3.5), ylab='', xlab='z-score', yaxt='n') ``` <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-9-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Standard Normal Distribution <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-10-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Standard Normal Distribution <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-11-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Standard Normal Distribution <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-12-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # What's the likelihood of ending with less than 15? ``` r pnorm(15, mean=mean(samples), sd=sd(samples)) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.9353996 ``` <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-14-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # What's the likelihood of ending with more than 15? ``` r 1 - pnorm(15, mean=mean(samples), sd=sd(samples)) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.06460038 ``` <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-16-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Comparing Scores on Different Scales SAT scores are distributed nearly normally with mean 1500 and standard deviation 300. ACT scores are distributed nearly normally with mean 21 and standard deviation 5. A college admissions officer wants to determine which of the two applicants scored better on their standardized test with respect to the other test takers: Pam, who earned an 1800 on her SAT, or Jim, who scored a 24 on his ACT? -- .pull-left[ ### Z-Scores * Z-scores are often called standard scores: $$ Z = \frac{observation - mean}{SD} $$ * Z-Scores have a mean = 0 and standard deviation = 1. ] -- .pull-right[ Converting Pam and Jim's scores to z-scores: $$ Z_{Pam} = \frac{1800 - 1500}{300} = 1 $$ $$ Z_{Jim} = \frac{24-21}{5} = 0.6 $$ ] --- class: font90 # Dual Scales <img src="images/hex/shiny.png" class="title-hex"> Some problems<sup>1</sup>: * The designer has to make choices about scales and this can have a big impact on the viewer * "Cross-over points” where one series cross another are results of the design choices, not intrinsic to the data, and viewers (particularly unsophisticated viewers) * They make it easier to lazily associate correlation with causation, not taking into account autocorrelation and other time-series issues * Because of the issues above, in malicious hands they make it possible to deliberately mislead This example looks at the relationship between NZ dollar exchange rate and trade weighted index. ``` r DATA606::shiny_demo('DualScales', package='DATA606') ``` My advise: * Avoid using them. You can usually do better with other plot types. * When necessary (or compelled) to use them, rescale (using z-scores, we'll discuss this in a few weeks) .font50[ <sup>1</sup> http://blog.revolutionanalytics.com/2016/08/dual-axis-time-series.html <sup>2</sup> http://ellisp.github.io/blog/2016/08/18/dualaxes ] --- # Standard Normal Parameters <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-18-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /><img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-18-2.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # SAT Variability SAT scores are distributed nearly normally with mean 1500 and standard deviation 300. * 68% of students score between 1200 and 1800 on the SAT. * 95% of students score between 900 and 2100 on the SAT. * 99.7% of students score between 600 and 2400 on the SAT. --- # Evaluating Normal Approximation To use the 68-95-99 rule, we must verify the normality assumption. We will want to do this also later when we talk about various (parametric) modeling. Consider a sample of 100 male heights (in inches). <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-20-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Evaluating Normal Approximation Histogram looks normal, but we can overlay a standard normal curve to help evaluation. <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-21-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Normal Q-Q Plot .pull-left[ <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-22-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> ] .pull-right[.font120[ * Data are plotted on the y-axis of a normal probability plot, and theoretical quantiles (following a normal distribution) on the x-axis. * If there is a linear relationship in the plot, then the data follow a nearly normal distribution. * Constructing a normal probability plot requires calculating percentiles and corresponding z-scores for each observation, which is tedious. Therefore we generally rely on software when making these plots. ]] --- # Skewness <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-23-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- # Simulated Normal Q-Q Plots ``` r DATA606::qqnormsim(heights) ``` <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-24-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> --- class: font120 # Milgram Experiment .pull-left[.font120[ * Stanley Milgram conducted a series of experiments on obedience to authority starting in 1963. * Experimenter (E) orders the teacher (T), the subject of the experiment, to give severe electric shocks to a learner (L) each time the learner answers a question incorrectly. ]] .pull-right[ <img src='images/milgram.png' height='500'> ] --- class: font130 # Milgram Experiment (cont.) * The learner is actually an actor, and the electric shocks are not real, but a prerecorded sound is played each time the teacher administers an electric shock. * These experiments measured the willingness of study participants to obey an authority figure who instructed them to perform acts that conflicted with their personal conscience. * Milgram found that about 65% of people would obey authority and give such shocks. * Over the years, additional research suggested this number is approximately consistent across communities and time. --- # Bernoulli Sequences * Each person in Milgram’s experiment can be thought of as a trial. * A person is labeled a success if she refuses to administer a severe shock, and failure if she administers such shock. * Since only 35% of people refused to administer a shock, probability of success is p = 0.35. * When an individual trial has only two possible outcomes, it is called a **Bernoulli** random variable. A random variable X has a *Bernoulli distribution* with parameter *p* if $$ P(X=1) = p \quad and \quad P(X=0) = 1 - p $$ for `\(0 < p < 1\)` --- # Geometric distribution Dr. Smith wants to repeat Milgrams experiments but she only wants to sample people until she finds someone who will not inflict a severe shock. What is the probability that she stops after the first person? `$$P(1^{st}\quad person\quad refuses) = 0.35$$` the third person? $$ P(1^{st} and 2^{nd} shock, 3^{rd} refuses) = \frac{S}{0.65} \times \frac{S}{0.65} \times \frac{R}{0.35} = 0.65^{2} \times 0.35 \approx 0.15 $$ the tenth person? --- # Geometric distribution (cont.) Geometric distribution describes the waiting time until a success for *independent and identically distributed* (iid) Bernouilli random variables. * independence: outcomes of trials don’t affect each other * identical: the probability of success is the same for each trial Geometric probabilities If `\(p\)` represents probability of success, `\((1 − p)\)` represents probability of failure, and n represents number of independent trials `$$P(success\quad on\quad the\quad { n }^{ th }\quad trial) = (1 − p)^{n−1}p$$` --- # Expected value How many people is Dr. Smith expected to test before finding the first one that refuses to administer the shock? The expected value, or the mean, of a geometric distribution is defined as `\(\frac{1}{p}\)`. $$ \mu = \frac{1}{p} = \frac{1}{0.35} = 2.86 $$ She is expected to test 2.86 people before finding the first one that refuses to administer the shock. But how can she test a non-whole number of people? --- # Expected value and its variability <center><table width='80%'><tr><td align='center'> $$ \mu = \frac{1}{p} $$ </td><td align='center'> $$ \sigma = \sqrt{\frac{1-p}{p^2}} $$ </td></tr></table></center> Going back to Dr. Smith’s experiment: $$ \sigma = \sqrt{\frac{1-p}{p^2}} = \sqrt{\frac{1-0.35}{0.35^2}} = 2.3 $$ Dr. Smith is expected to test 2.86 people before finding the first one that refuses to administer the shock, give or take 2.3 people. These values only make sense in the context of repeating the experiment many many times. --- # Milgram Part 2 Suppose we randomly select four individuals to participate in this experiment. What is the probability that exactly 1 of them will refuse to administer the shock Let’s call these people Allen (A), Brittany (B), Caroline (C), and Damian (D). Each one of the four scenarios below will satisfy the condition of “exactly 1 of them refuses to administer the shock”: .center[ ![](images/milgram2.png) ] The probability of exactly one 1 of 4 people refusing to administer the shock is the sum of all of these probabilities. 0.0961 + 0.0961 + 0.0961 + 0.0961 = 4 × 0.0961 = 0.3844 --- # Binomial distribution The question from the prior slide asked for the probability of given number of successes, k, in a given number of trials, n, (k = 1 success in n = 4 trials), and we calculated this probability as `$$# of scenarios × P(single scenario)$$` Number of scenarios: there is a less tedious way to figure this out, we’ll get to that shortly... `$$P(single \quad scenario) = p^k (1 − p)^{(n−k)}$$` The *Binomial* distribution describes the probability of having exactly k successes in n independent Bernouilli trials with probability of success p. --- # Choose Function The choose function is useful for calculating the number of ways to choose k successes in *n* trials. $$ \left( \begin{matrix} n \\\\ k \end{matrix} \right) =\frac { n! }{ k!(n-k)! } $$ For example, : $$ \left( \begin{matrix} 9 \\\\ 2 \end{matrix} \right) =\frac { 9! }{ 2!(9-2)! } =\frac { 9\times 8\times 7! }{ 2\times 1\times 7! } =\frac { 72 }{ 2 } =36 $$ ``` r choose(9,2) ``` ``` ## [1] 36 ``` --- # Binomial distribution If p represents probability of success, (1 − p) represents probability of failure, n represents number of independent trials, and k represents number of successes $$ P(k\quad successes\quad in\quad n\quad trials)=\left( \begin{matrix} n \\\\ k \end{matrix} \right) { p }^{ k }{ (1-p) }^{ (n-k) } $$ --- # Binomial distribution ``` r n <- 4 p <- 0.35 barplot(dbinom(0:n, n, p), names.arg=0:n) ``` <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-26-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> ``` r dbinom(1, 4, p) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.384475 ``` https://shiny.rit.albany.edu/stat/binomial/ --- class: left, font140 # One Minute Paper .pull-left[ 1. What was the most important thing you learned during this class? 2. What important question remains unanswered for you? ] .pull-right[ <img src="04-Distributions_files/figure-html/unnamed-chunk-27-1.png" style="display: block; margin: auto;" /> ] https://forms.gle/ESBAdHRhzT65fW6c6